Attachment
Behavior: The Causes and Manifestations of Secure and Insecure Attachments
In an effort to evaluate the relationship
between infants and caregivers, researchers developed a laboratory method
termed the “Strange Situation.” Using the Strange Situation, infants are
introduced to strangers, separated from caregivers and reunited with
caregivers. Infants were then classified into different attachment categories
depending on their reactions to the strangers and to their caregivers (Berk,
2012, p. 266).
The term “attachment” refers to the
intimate relationships between individuals and relationship partners. However, sometimes
support by a relationship partner is unavailable or unsympathetic when needed.
This can cause individual to start experiencing emotional troubles and
self-doubt and develop attachment anxiety (Mikulincer, Shaver, Sapir-Lavid, &
Avihou-Kanza, 2009, p. 615). In an appropriate relationship between mother and
infant, the infant can rely on the mother for feelings of security. The infant
then feels safe to explore the environment as long as the mother is nearby
(Cicchetti, Rogosch & Toth, 2006, p. 624). The way a parent chooses to
spend time with a child is less applicable in terms of attachment than general
responsiveness and availability. Attachment can be predicted to a degree before
the child is even born and attachment is evidently more a result of behavior
than genetics (Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 66).
In
the Strange Situation, securely attached children may display some distress
when the parent leaves and they exhibit positive reactions upon his or her
return. When distressed, a secure child looks for comfort from the parent and is
easily soothed by the parent. Secure children also expect caregivers to be
responsive (Berk, 2012, p. 266).
There
are three aspects that make up a secure attachment between an individual and a
relationship partner: “(a) If one encounters an obstacle and/or becomes
distressed, one can approach a relationship partner for help; (b) this partner
will be available and supportive when one asks for support; and (c) one will
experience relief and comfort as a result of proximity to this person” (Mikulincer
et. al, 2009, p. 615). Individuals who are securely attached can rely on
receiving support from partners when they need it. They do not need to use
defense mechanisms that can hurt themselves or their relationships with partners.
They are optimistic about the reactions of partners, do not worry about being
treated negatively, and are able to be open about intimate issues. Relationship
partners in securely attached relationships enforce the idea that individuals
are worthy of time and attention (Mikulincer et. al, 2009, p. 616). Securely
attached individuals cope more easily with stress and are more likely to look
for alternatives to problems rather than focus only on possible negative
results. These individuals are also more likely to notice positive behaviors
about others and are more likely to forgive occasional errant behavior (Mikulincer
et. al, 2009, p. 631).
Sensitive
parental responsiveness includes prompt response to a child’s distress and also
being generally open to the attentions of the infant (Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 66).
Mothers of infants who are securely attached are also more likely to be
sensitive to their children during play (Fuertes, Lopes-dos-Santos, Beeghly,
& Tronick, 2009, p. 328). In addition, secure children are better able to
interact with peers (Fagot, 1997, p. 490). It is possible that children who are
securely attached will approach strange peers differently than children who did
not develop a secure attachment and thus receive a more positive response. Secure
children are also seen as less dependent. They are more likely to make friends
and participate longer in group activities (Fagot, 1997, p. 497).
Unlike
securely attached children, children who are described as having an avoidant
attachment generally treat the parent and the stranger in the same fashion as
seen in the Strange Situation. They do not display positive reactions when the
mother returns and they are not likely to cling (Berk, 2012, p. 267). Infants with avoidant attachments not
only treat caregivers with no more warmth than a stranger, but also initially
avoid them at a time of reunion (Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 66). Research has
shown that mothers of these infants do not provide a comforting touch, are
often angry, and intrude upon the infant without regard for the infant’s need.
These infants have learned to manage their distress by not displaying it and
instead focusing on nearby objects, though they may remain anxious. They also
stay away from anything that may heighten their distress, since they understand
that a comforting touch will not be available to them to lessen that distress
(Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 67). Insecure-avoidant infants are more likely to close
their eyes, mouth objects or look away from their mothers while their mothers
are associating with them. Mothers of infants who are classified as having
avoidant attachments are more likely to display controlling behavior during
play (Fuertes et al., 2009, p. 328). Avoidant children may also learn to
withdraw as a result of being over stimulated at inappropriate times (Berk,
2012, p. 270).
When
assessing the ability of children to overcome obstacles, Fagot (1997) observed
that children with avoidant personalities “had fewer object struggles than did
secure children.” In addition, girls with avoidant personalities have more
trouble with classmates than girls with secure attachments. Avoidant children
are more likely to use avoidance or act defensively in their interactions with
others. However, even though avoidant children may remain withdrawn and
struggle with personal relationships, they may still develop the ability to
give favorable public presentations (p. 490).
Avoidant
personality disorder (AvPD) is a condition given to those with avoidant
attachments. People who are diagnosed with AvPD are more anxious about negative
responses from others and experience lower levels of self-esteem. AvPD is
closely related to social phobia (SP) (Eggum, Eisenberg, Spinrad, Valiente, Edwards, Kupfer, & Reiser, 2009,
p. 815). Children with stronger behavioral inhibitions may be more likely to
exhibit AvPD, because they are less likely to participate in novel activities (Eggum et al., 2009, p. 816). AvPD and SP are
both related to negative emotionality, and fearfulness may be a cause of
avoidant behavior. Depression and anger are also related to AvPD and SP, though
it is unclear whether they are causes or results (Eggum et al., 2009, p. 819). In addition
to low feelings of self-worth, AvPD has also been linked to strong feelings of pessimism
(Eggum et al., 2009, p.
820). Individuals seeking emotional treatment, such as those with AvPD, usually
reported being abused (whether physically or emotionally) and/or neglected
during childhood. Avoidant individuals are also likely to have a combination of
genetics and childhood environment that encouraged the disorder (Eggum et al., 2009, p. 832). Aspects
related to shyness can be due to avoidant withdrawal, but they can also be the
result of temperament (Eggum
et al., 2009, p. 833).
People
who develop avoidant attachment distrust the motives of their closest
acquaintances and end up distancing themselves as a result (Mikulincer et. al, 2009,
p. 616). In addition, individuals
who experience avoidant attachment are likely to extend their feelings of
avoidance to their spouses, resulting in less marital commitment and more cases
of infidelity. They have already learned to rely only upon themselves, thereby
avoiding feelings of dependence on a partner that a healthy marriage requires (DeWall,
Lambert, Slotter, Pond, Deckman, Finkel, Luchies, & Fincham, 2011, p. 1304). People with avoidant attachment
display more positive attitudes about infidelity and also pay attention to
attractive alternatives (DeWall et al., 2011, p. 1313). However, they do not
seem to commit infidelity in order to hurt a partner. They simply seem to lack
the inhibition that people with secure attachments are more likely to have and
follow in order to stay in happy relationships (DeWall et al., 2011, p. 1314).
Another form of attachment is resistant
attachment. When describing resistant attachment as it relates to the Strange
Situation, Berk (2012) asserts that resistant children do not feel safe leaving
their mothers and may not be easily calmed. They often show angry, aggressive
behavior (p. 267). Resistant children generally have unresponsive caregivers
(Berk, 2012, p. 270). These infants also are more likely to displays behaviors
such as crying, pushing or turning away while being held by their mothers.
Mothers of infants who display resistant attachment are more likely to be
unresponsive to their infants during play (Fuertes et al., 2009, p. 328). These
mothers are also more likely to have lower levels of education (Fuertes et al.,
2009, p. 327).
In evaluation resistant children in
social settings, it is evident they are less likely to be approached by peers.
They are also more likely to use coercive methods and may try to draw attention
to themselves through clowning around or tattle telling. They are also less
able than secure children to maintain relationships (Fagot, 1997, p. 490).
Positive gestures from resistant and avoidant children are more likely to be
met with negative gestures from other children. However, children of avoidant
and resistant personalities will have different methods of interacting with
peers (Fagot, 1997, p. 495). When
Fagot evaluated the association between children and their peers, he learned
that securely attached and insecurely attached children, even though their
gestures may be similar, are likely to receive different responses from peers (Fagot,
1997, p. 495). However, secure attachment does not guarantee positive
relationships with peers (Fagot, 1997, p. 496).
The last method of attachment listed by
Berk (2012) in regards to the Strange Situation is disorganized/disoriented
attachment. It is characterized by infants who “show confused, contradictory
behaviors—for example, looking away while the parent is holding them or
approaching the parent with flat, depressed emotion. Most display a dazed
facial expression, and a few cry out unexpectedly after having calmed down or
display odd, frozen postures” (p. 267). Disorganized attachment refers to
attachment behavior where the behavior of the parent is unpredictable
(Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 67). Children who develop disorganized-disoriented
attachments are likely to have caregivers who developed insecure attachments as
children themselves with their own caregivers (Cicchetti et al., 2006, p. 625).
Aggressive behavior has been linked to
disorganized attachment. Aggressive behavior is divided into two different
categories: “oppositional defiant disorder (ODD) and conduct disorder (CD).”
ODD is generally a prerequisite to CD and often will be witnessed in children
first. ODD includes behaviors of disobedience, backtalk and tantrums. CD
includes behaviors such as arson, fighting and stealing (Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p.
64). Children with aggressive temperaments are likely to raise their own
children the same way they had been raised, often involving coercive behavior.
Mothers of aggressive children are more likely to blame their children’s personalities
for behavioral problems and less likely to recognize the problems in their own behavior
(Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 65). Both boys and girls with mothers who are hostile are
more likely to exhibit aggressive behavior. Their mothers often do not live with
a partner. Children with disorganized attachment patterns are likely to exhibit
helpless or depressed behavior. (Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 67). Unlike securely
attached children who are surprised when caregivers are unresponsive,
insecurely attached children (including those with disorganized attachments) do
not even recognize a difference between responsive and unresponsive caregivers
(Berk, 2012, p. 266).
Children with disorganized attachment
are also likely to experience continued high levels of stress after being
assessed, whereas avoidant children had intermediate levels of stress and
secure children were relatively relaxed after assessment. In evaluating the
parents of disorganized children, it is seen that over sixty percent of
children of adolescent mothers have shown “disorganized attachment patterns” (Lyons-Ruth,
1996, p. 68). It is likely that a child develops a disorganized attachment
after failing in trying to establish a predictable relationship with his or her
caregiver (Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 69). Mothers of infants with disorganized
attachment are also less likely to have a positive perspective regarding their
infants’ temperaments (Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 70) and children with disorganized
attachment are likely to have been maltreated (Berk, 2012, p. 270).
About fifteen percent of disorganized
infants come from middle-class two-parent families. However, in cases where
mothers have depression, drink alcohol or are adolescent, the number of
children with disorganized attachment climbs to over eighty percent. On the
other hand, infants with physical disabilities are not more likely to attain a
disorganized attachment pattern (Lyons-Ruth, 1996, p. 67).
Concerning insecure people in general, babies
who are unable to form an intimate bond with an adult develop emotional
problems. They are also less able to interpret facial expressions (Berk, 2012,
p. 269). Insecure infants are likely to be held less affectionately and to be
treated with resentment. Difficult babies and preterm babies with depressed
mothers are more likely to develop insecure attachments (Berk, 2012, p. 271).
Less-secure individuals are more likely to misunderstand caring behavior by
partners and are more likely to magnify their own need, resulting in greater
distress. They also see and dream about people needing support and relief from
distress, but unable to find it (Mikulincer et. al, 2009, p. 631).
Proper
care giving for a child involves providing for the child’s physical, emotional
and educational needs. Children who are maltreated generally experience
physical or emotional abuse (Cicchetti et al., 2006, p. 627). They are likely
to develop mental and physical issues as they grow older. For this reason, it
is important to intervene in cases where children have been maltreated (Cicchetti
et al., 2006, p. 624). Therapists have been able to help mothers differentiate
between their own childhood experiences from those the mothers have with their
children. Therapists also helped mothers develop empathy with their infants (Cicchetti
et al., 2006, p. 630). Mothers who had maltreated children are more likely to
have experienced abuse and neglect in their own childhoods and are more likely
to still be angry with their own mothers (Cicchetti et al., 2006, p. 635).
Two
methods of intervention for maltreated infants dramatically increased the
number of infants who were securely attached and greatly reduced the number of
infants who experienced disorganized attachment (Cicchetti et al., 2006, pp. 638-639).
The first method involved training the mother in child development, parenting,
stress management and seeking out social support. The second method changed how
negative mother-child models affected current relationships (Cicchetti et al.,
2006, p. 625). Mothers with maltreated infants have less available family
support and feel less competent as a parent. They also expressed ignorance in
knowing how to raise a child (Cicchetti
et al., 2006, p. 642). On the
other hand, “infants who have been maltreated are highly likely to maintain
disorganized/dis-oriented attachments in the absence of intensive efforts to
improve the mother–child relationship and parenting” (Cicchetti et al.,
2006, p. 644).
The study yielded positive results in the
knowledge that mother-child relationships can be greatly improved. On the other
hand, the study also displays sobering evidence that children with disorganized
attachments are at great risk of maladaptive development if intervention does
not occur (Cicchetti et al., 2006, p. 645). However, successful intervention likely relies
on knowledgeable, experienced therapists (Cicchetti et al., 2006, p. 644).
Overall, secure attachments are more
likely to persist than insecure attachments. In some cultures where mothers are
quick to respond to the needs of the infant, avoidant attachment is nonexistent
(Berk, 2012, p. 268). These studies explain the definitions and causes of
various forms of attachment as well as outlining the benefits of secure
attachment and difficulties of insecure attachments.
References
Berk,
L. E. (2012). Infants and Children:
Prenatal through middle childhood. (7th ed.). Boston, MA: Allyn
& Bacon.
Cicchetti, D., Rogosch, F. A., & Toth, S. L. (2006). Fostering
secure attachment in infants in maltreating families through preventive
interventions. Development and Psychopathology, 18(3), 623-49. http://search.proquest.com/docview/201698289?accountid=8289
DeWall,
C. N., Lambert, N. M., Slotter, E. B., Pond, R. S., Deckman, T., Finkel, E. J.,
Luchies, L. B., & Fincham, F. D. (2011). So far away from ones partner, yet
so close to romantic alternatives: Avoidant attachment, interest in
alternatives, and infidelity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology,
101(6), 1302-1316. doi:10.1037/a0025497
Eggum,
N. D., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Valiente, C., Edwards, A., Kupfer, A. S.,
& Reiser, M. (2009). Predictors of withdrawal: Possible precursors of
avoidant personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology, 21(3),
815-38. doi:10.1017/S0954579409000443
Fagot,
B. I. (1997). Attachment, parenting, and peer interactions of toddler children.
Developmental Psychology, 33(3), 489-499. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.33.3.489
Fuertes, M., Lopes-dos-Santos, P., Beeghly, M., &
Tronick, E. (2009). Infant coping and maternal interactive
behavior predict attachment in a portuguese sample of healthy preterm infants.
European Psychologist, 14(4), 320-331. doi:10.1027/1016-9040.14.4.320
Mikulincer,
M., Shaver, P. R., Sapir-Lavid, Y., & Avihou-Kanza, N. (2009). What’s
inside the minds of securely and insecurely attached people? The secure-base
script and its associations with attachment-style dimensions. Journal of
Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4), 615-633. doi:10.1037/a0015649
Lyons-Ruth,
K. (1996). Attachment relationships among children with aggressive behavior
problems: The role of disorganized early attachment patterns. Journal of
Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 64(1), 64-73.
doi:10.1037/0022-006X.64.1.64
No comments:
Post a Comment